Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 6 de 6
Filtrar
Mais filtros










Base de dados
Intervalo de ano de publicação
1.
J Exp Child Psychol ; 228: 105609, 2023 04.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-36587438

RESUMO

Young children share equally when they acquire resources through collaboration with a partner, yet it is unclear whether they do so because in such contexts resources are encountered as common and distributed in front of the recipient or because collaboration promotes a sense of work-based fairness. In the current studies, 5- and 8-year-old children from Germany (N = 193) acquired resources either by working individually alongside or by collaborating with a peer. After finding out that the partner's container was empty, they decided in private whether they wanted to donate some resources to the peer. When both partners had worked with equal efforts (Study 1), children shared more after collaboration than after individual work. When one partner had worked with much more effort than the other (Study 2), children shared more with a harder-working partner than with a less-working partner independently of whether they had collaborated or worked individually. Younger children were more generous than older children, in particular after collaboration. These findings support the view that collaboration promotes a genuine sense of fairness in young children, but they also indicate that merit-based notions of fairness in the context of work may develop independently of collaboration, at least by the beginning of middle childhood and in Western societies.


Assuntos
Comportamento Cooperativo , Grupo Associado , Humanos , Criança , Adolescente , Pré-Escolar , Alemanha , Atividade Motora
2.
Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A ; 119(1)2022 01 04.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34969840

RESUMO

Individuals in all societies conform to their cultural group's conventional norms, from how to dress on certain occasions to how to play certain games. It is an open question, however, whether individuals in all societies actively enforce the group's conventional norms when others break them. We investigated third-party enforcement of conventional norms in 5- to 8-y-old children (n = 376) from eight diverse small-scale and large-scale societies. Children learned the rules for playing a new sorting game and then, observed a peer who was apparently breaking them. Across societies, observer children intervened frequently to correct their misguided peer (i.e., more frequently than when the peer was following the rules). However, both the magnitude and the style of interventions varied across societies. Detailed analyses of children's interactions revealed societal differences in children's verbal protest styles as well as in their use of actions, gestures, and nonverbal expressions to intervene. Observers' interventions predicted whether their peer adopted the observer's sorting rule. Enforcement of conventional norms appears to be an early emerging human universal that comes to be expressed in culturally variable ways.


Assuntos
Desenvolvimento Infantil , Identificação Social , Normas Sociais , Criança , Pré-Escolar , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino
4.
Nat Commun ; 9(1): 2076, 2018 05 25.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29802252

RESUMO

Social information use is a pivotal characteristic of the human species. Avoiding the cost of individual exploration, social learning confers substantial fitness benefits under a wide variety of environmental conditions, especially when the process is governed by biases toward relative superiority (e.g., experts, the majority). Here, we examine the development of social information use in children aged 4-14 years (n = 605) across seven societies in a standardised social learning task. We measured two key aspects of social information use: general reliance on social information and majority preference. We show that the extent to which children rely on social information depends on children's cultural background. The extent of children's majority preference also varies cross-culturally, but in contrast to social information use, the ontogeny of majority preference follows a U-shaped trajectory across all societies. Our results demonstrate both cultural continuity and diversity in the realm of human social learning.


Assuntos
Características Culturais , Diversidade Cultural , Comportamento Social , Meio Social , Aprendizado Social/fisiologia , Adolescente , Desenvolvimento do Adolescente/fisiologia , Criança , Desenvolvimento Infantil/fisiologia , Pré-Escolar , Comparação Transcultural , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino
5.
Psychol Sci ; 26(8): 1252-60, 2015 Aug.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26115962

RESUMO

Distributing the spoils of a joint enterprise on the basis of work contribution or relative productivity seems natural to the modern Western mind. But such notions of merit-based distributive justice may be culturally constructed norms that vary with the social and economic structure of a group. In the present research, we showed that children from three different cultures have very different ideas about distributive justice. Whereas children from a modern Western society distributed the spoils of a joint enterprise precisely in proportion to productivity, children from a gerontocratic pastoralist society in Africa did not take merit into account at all. Children from a partially hunter-gatherer, egalitarian African culture distributed the spoils more equally than did the other two cultures, with merit playing only a limited role. This pattern of results suggests that some basic notions of distributive justice are not universal intuitions of the human species but rather culturally constructed behavioral norms.


Assuntos
Comparação Transcultural , Recompensa , Justiça Social , Criança , Feminino , Alemanha , Humanos , Quênia , Masculino , Namíbia
6.
Psychol Sci ; 20(5): 654-60, 2009 May.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-19476595

RESUMO

One of the defining features of human language is displacement, the ability to make reference to absent entities. Here we show that prelinguistic, 12-month-old infants already can use a nonverbal pointing gesture to make reference to absent entities. We also show that chimpanzees-who can point for things they want humans to give them-do not point to refer to absent entities in the same way. These results demonstrate that the ability to communicate about absent but mutually known entities depends not on language, but rather on deeper social-cognitive skills that make acts of linguistic reference possible in the first place. These nonlinguistic skills for displaced reference emerged apparently only after humans' divergence from great apes some 6 million years ago.


Assuntos
Comunicação Animal , Desenvolvimento da Linguagem , Rememoração Mental , Comunicação não Verbal , Pan troglodytes/psicologia , Psicologia da Criança , Animais , Atenção , Evolução Biológica , Feminino , Gestos , Humanos , Lactente , Masculino , Teoria da Construção Pessoal , Comportamento Social
SELEÇÃO DE REFERÊNCIAS
DETALHE DA PESQUISA
...